Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Distance-based functional criticality in the human brain: intelligence and emotional intelligence

Abstract

Background

Anatomical distance has been identified as a key factor in the organizational principles of the human brain. On the other hand, criticality was proposed to accommodate the multiscale properties of human brain dynamics, and functional criticality based on resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rfMRI) is a sensitive neuroimaging marker for human brain dynamics. Hence, to explore the effects of anatomical distance of the human brain on behaviors in terms of functional criticality, we proposed a revised algorithm of functional criticality called the distance-based vertex-wise index of functional criticality, and assessed this algorithm compared with the original neighborhood-based functional criticality.

Results

We recruited two groups of healthy participants, including young adults and middle-aged participants, for a total of 60 datasets including rfMRI and intelligence as well as emotional intelligence to study how human brain functional criticalities at different spatial scales contribute to individual behaviors. Furthermore, we defined the average distance between the particular behavioral map and vertices with significant functional connectivity as connectivity distance. Our results demonstrated that intelligence and emotional intelligence mapped to different brain regions at different ages. Additionally, intelligence was related to a wider distance range compared to emotional intelligence.

Conclusions

For different age groups, our findings not only provided a linkage between intelligence/emotional intelligence and functional criticality but also quantitatively characterized individual behaviors in terms of anatomical distance.

Background

Understanding how the human brain works is a major challenge for neuroscientists and especially computational neuroscientists. The cerebral cortex has been intensively proven to be modularly organized, and different modules have been gradually identified with boundaries that are related to different cognitive functions using functional magnetic resonance imaging [1,2,3,4]. Together with interactions between the modules mediated by numerous pathways within the cerebral architecture, scientists have basically accepted the concept that the human brain works as a whole-brain network. DTI and fMRI permit the analysis of cortical structural and functional networks at relatively high spatial and temporal resolution [5, 6]. Consistent with graph theory, many organizational principles of cerebral cortex architecture, such as the existence of network hubs and small worldness, have been proposed [7]. However, these data-driven empirical observations did not abstract a simple and quantitative characterization of brain network organizational principles until Ercsey-Ravasz et al. [8] testified that anatomical distance between cortical regions played an important role in macaque brain network organization. These researchers reported that connection weights exponentially decay with the interareal distance, and a single-parameter random graph model based on this rule successfully predicted numerous features of the cortical network.

Coincidentally, physical distance has also been shown to be an influential factor in human brain architecture using traditional resting-state fMRI techniques. The material and metabolic cost of long distances results in decreased internode associations, and this distance-dependent functional profile has been proposed and tested for approximately one decade. Salvador et al. [9] summarized an inverse square law describing the dependence of functional connectivity on anatomical distance. These distance-dependent observations in human brain functional architecture added evidence to Ercsey-Ravasz’s model and inspired our current studies on distance-based functional criticality of the human brain. In addition, numerous studies have addressed different roles of local and distant functional connectivity discriminated by anatomical distance in healthy and unhealthy brain functional organization: local and distant functional interactions have been proven to indicate cortical hierarchical organization [10]; different distributions of long and short connectivity are key neuropathologies of multiple neuropsychiatric diseases [11,12,13,14]. Although local and distant connectivity play important roles in human brain function, no study has integrated them into a single theoretical framework to study human brain functional organization.

Network theory has been shown to be useful in characterizing human brain functional organization rules. The human brain requires the coordination of neural activity across many spatial and temporal scales, ranging from neurons and circuits to large-scale networks. Criticality is scale free and could accommodate this multiscale phenomenon in the human brain. Additionally, studies have demonstrated that the human brain works near criticality to accomplish the transitions of task states [15,16,17]. By deriving the nonlinear dynamics of human brain equations and the neighborhood hypothesis, we previously proposed vertexwise functional criticality to study criticality in healthy brains and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) progression [18,19,20]. Now that distance was a key factor in human brain network organization, in this study, we proposed the distance-based vertexwise Index of Functional Criticality (D-vIFC) instead of the neighborhood-based index. Our new algorithm not only originated from critical theory in nonlinear dynamics but also took distance into account and integrated both local and distant connectivity in the human brain architecture.

Intelligence could predict one’s overall level of career achievement and quality of life. It is a very integrated concept and generally involves verbal, performance and social intelligence [21]. Hence, narrowly speaking, intelligence and emotional intelligence dissociate humans from other animals, and they are the two most well-known traits for personal achievements. Many brain imaging studies have confirmed the importance of the frontoparietal network for intelligence [22, 23]. Compared with intelligence, emotional intelligence has been more debate on its construct with less evidence of brain mechanisms. Emotional intelligence is similar to social cognition [24] and largely associated with the amygdala. Jausovec and Jausovec [21] also reported that intelligence (verbal and performance) and emotional intelligence were related to different electrophysiological signals. However, no fMRI studies have addressed intelligence and emotional intelligence together. Given that the prefrontal cortex was proposed to interplay between emotion and cognition [25, 26], does cognitive intelligence overlap with emotional intelligence in the context of brain mechanisms? Do brain mechanisms of intelligence and emotional intelligence interact with age? Are these mechanisms encoded by functional criticalities of different spatial scales within human brain functional architecture?

In this study, we proposed a new algorithm of rfMRI-derived human brain functional criticality based on interareal distance, D-vIFC. This algorithm used a distance-dependent definition of the boundary of the dominant cluster in human brain functional architecture, combining both local and distant functional connectivity of the human brain. Using 60 datasets, including rfMRI, intelligence and emotional intelligence scores, we aimed to study whether associations exist between functional criticality D-vIFC and behaviors as well as age-related interactions. Additionally, we defined a new measurement of brain connectivity, namely, connectivity distance, aiming to validate the distance-dependent D-vIFC associations of intelligence and emotional intelligence and to provide a quantitative characterization for individual behaviors in psychology.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-seven healthy subjects (32 males, aged 18.6–64.3) were recruited from the local community or universities by advertisements. All the participants were invited for a detailed mental health interview using the Mini-International Neuro-Psychiatric Interview. Individuals with a history of major neuropsychiatric illness, head injury, alcohol and drug abuse were excluded. Participants were assessed with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-4th Edition (in Chinese, WAIS-IV), Schutte Self-Report Emotional Intelligence Scale in Chinese Version (SSEIS), State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Mental Health Continuum-Short Form, Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, Chinese Perceived Stress Scale, Achievement Motivation Scale, and Self-Control Scale. The institutional review board of the Institute of Psychology Chinese Academy of Sciences approved this study, and written informed consent was obtained from individual participants prior to data acquisition.

Behaviour measures

The WAIS-IV was used to measure cognitive intelligence. The full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) is a composite score obtained from 10 subtests measuring two components of cognitive abilities: general ability and cognitive proficiency. The general ability index (GAI) comprises two subindices: the verbal comprehension index (VCI) and perceptual reasoning index (PRI). The cognitive proficiency index (CPI) comprises two subindices: the working memory index (WMI) and processing speed index (PSI).

The SSEIS was applied to measure emotional intelligence. It is a valid assessment developed by Schutte et al. [27] and originates from the emotional intelligence model of Salovey and Mayer [28]. The Chinese version of the SSEIS exhibits high reliability and validity and consists of 33 items that are assessed using a 5-point Likert scale to measure four dimensions, including emotion perception, emotion management of the self, emotion management of others, and emotion utilization [29]. Participants were asked to respond to each item: ‘1’ represented ‘not true of me’ and ‘5’ represented ‘very true of me’. Finally, the average score of all items was just the total score of emotional intelligence. The Cronbach’s α in the present study was 0.90.

MRI imaging

All the MRI images were collected on the 3.0 T GE scanner Discovery MR750 at the Institute of Psychology Chinese Academy of Sciences. All the participants completed a T1-weighted structural MRI scan (eyes closed) with an ABI1_t1iso_fspgr sequence (TR = 6.652 ms; TE = 2.928 ms; FA = 12°; matrix = 256 × 256; slice thickness = 1 mm) and an 8-min resting-state fMRI scan (eyes open with a fixation cross) using a gradient echo EPI sequence ABI1_bold_bw_rest (TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; FA = 90°; number of slices = 33 (interleaved); slice thickness = 3.5 mm; gap = 0.7 mm; and matrix = 64 × 64).

Imaging data preprocessing

All the images were preprocessed using the Connectome Computation System (CCS), which was formulated by our lab using FSL, AFNI and FreeSurfer [30]. Its distinctive characteristic is that it focuses on surface-based analysis compared to other resting-state fMRI data analysis pipelines. The system combines anatomical, structural and functional information to provide a computational platform for brain connectome analysis with multimodal neuroimaging data [31]. Preprocessing is composed of structural image preprocessing and functional image preprocessing, and the main purposes of the preprocessing stage are (1) to remove irrelevant brain tissues, including the skull, cerebrospinal fluid, and white matter; (2) to reduce the noise of the MRI images and (3) to facilitate registration across all the participants for the final group analysis. Structural image preprocessing was conducted surrounding cortical surface reconstruction [32, 33], which included (1) T1 image noise removal and brain extraction using the volBrain automated volumetry system (https://www.volbrain.upv.es) [34]; (2) segmentation of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), white matter (WM) and gray matter (GM), construction of the GM-WM (white surface) and GM-CSF interface (pial surface), and (3) spatial registration by matching of the cortical folding patterns across subjects by recon-all in FreeSurfer. The functional image preprocessing involved more procedures: the first 5 EPI volumes (10 s) are removed to allow for signal equilibration, removal and interpolation of temporal spikes, slice timing correction, alignment of each volume to a ‘base’ volume (the first EPI), normalization of the 4D global mean intensity into 10,000, motion-artifact removal using ICA-AROMA, correction of WM/CSF signals and the Friston-24 motion parameters [35, 36], bandpass (0.01–0.1 Hz) filtering, removal of both linear and quadratic trends, and alignment of the individual functional image to the anatomical image with a GM-WM boundary-based registration algorithm [37]. Finally, individual preprocessed 4D rfMRI time series were projected onto the fsaverage5 standard cortical surface with 10,242 vertices per hemisphere and an average spacing of approximately 4 mm [38]. Figure 1 shows a simplified pipeline for the data analysis strategy we used in this study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Pipelines for the data analysis strategies used in this study. All the data included structural MRI, functional MRI and behaviors. The data analysis comprised MRI data preprocessing, the vIFC algorithm (the flowchart), GLM statistics and connectivity distance

Quality control

Quality control plays key roles in making data analysis solid. Here, we considered the following steps for quality control: (1) brain extraction, (2) pial and white surface reconstruction, (3) boundary-based functional image registration, and (4) head motion correction. For the first three procedures, we acquired screenshots and checked their qualities by visual assessment. Quantitative controls of boundary-based functional image registration (mcBBR ≤ 0.65) and head motion (meanFD < 0.3 mm) were also used. One participant did not complete MRI scanning, and one participant did not pass the mental health interview. Five participants were excluded because their mcBBR was greater than 0.65. One participant underwent only the emotional intelligence test but did not undergo the intelligence test. Therefore, 59 participants were included in the intelligence analysis, and 60 participants were included in the emotional intelligence analysis. Considering the wide age range of our participants, we divided all the participants into two groups: the young adult group (aged 19.5–32.8) and the middle-aged group (aged 36.6–64.3 for intelligence and aged 35.9–64.3 for emotional intelligence). Detailed participant information and behavioral measurements are summarized in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1 Participant information. For the young adults, the same participants (17 males and 14 females) were assessed for intelligence and emotional intelligence. For the middle-aged adults, 28 participants were included in the intelligence assessment, and 29 participants were included in the emotional intelligence assessment
Table 2 Behavioral measurements of all the participants (N = 60)

D-vIFC algorithm

vIFC has been proposed based on nonlinear dynamical theory as an efficient neuroimaging marker that indicates probabilities that a critical transition occurs in the absence of knowledge of the details of realistic network connections [18]. In more detail, vIFC was designed to integrate three properties of the center manifold (subnetwork or a group of variables) in the abstract phase space of the complicated human brain network: increased within-group (dominant cluster) correlations, increased temporal variations and decreased between-group correlations. The neighborhood-based vIFC algorithm has been successfully used in normal and abnormal populations [19, 20]. In this study, we used inter-areal distance instead of neighborhood to define the boundary of the dominant cluster within the entire network architecture. The mathematical formula was the same as the original vIFC and is provided below:

$${\text{vIFC}}\left( i \right) = \frac{{STD\left( i \right)PCC_{in} }}{{PCC_{out} }} = \frac{{\sqrt {\mathop \sum \nolimits_{n = 1}^{N} \left( {x_{i} \left( {t_{n} } \right) - \left\langle {x_{i} \left( {t_{n} } \right)} \right\rangle } \right)^{2} } \cdot \left\langle {PCC_{ij} \left( {j \in I} \right)} \right\rangle }}{{\left\langle {PCC_{ik} \left( {k \notin I} \right)} \right\rangle }}$$
(1)

As illustrated in Eq. 1, there are three types of vertices, i.e., {i}, \(\left\{ {{\text{j}} \in {\text{I}}} \right\}\), and \(\left\{ {{\text{k}} \notin {\text{I}}} \right\}\). We chose four distance thresholds (D1 = 14 mm, vIFC1; D2 = 28 mm, vIFC2; D3 = 42 mm, vIFC3; D4 = 56 mm, vIFC4) to explore the associations between vIFC and behaviors at different spatial scales of brain connectivity. Figure 1 depicts D1 (vIFC1) and D2 (vIFC2) as examples. Considering that Sepulcre et al. [10] used 0.25 as a threshold of different distance-based functional connectivity networks, we calculated a Pearson correlation connectivity network for each vertex i using connectivity thresholds p1 = 0.15, p2 = 0.2, p3 = 0.25 and p4 = 0.3. Then, for given vertex i, I represents all the vertices that have shorter distances (connecting to i) than the distance threshold, j represents vertices within I, and k represents all the vertices that have longer distances (connecting to i) than the distance threshold within the entire connectivity network. According to the flowchart in Fig. 1, ‘in’ and ‘out’ represent the vertices inside and outside the sphere, respectively, with the radius of the distance threshold. N is the number of fMRI BOLD time points. xi(tn) stands for the fMRI BOLD value of the vertex i at time tn; PCC stands for the inter-vertex Pearson correlation coefficient across time; STD stands for the standard deviation of the BOLD time series; and <> in the ‘PCC’ means averaging across vertices. The calculation was repeated for each vertex, and we then obtained a D-vIFC map on the fsaverage5 surface of each participant. Considering that the distance thresholds were 14 mm, 28 mm, 42 mm and 56 mm, the vIFC maps were spatially smoothed with Gaussian kernels of 10 mm, 14 mm and 18 mm on fsaverage5.

Statistics

We performed statistical analysis to study the associations between vIFC maps (vIFC1, vIFC2, vIFC3, vIFC4) and behavioral measurements (intelligence and emotional intelligence) as shown in Fig. 1. We employed FreeSurfer Group Descriptor (FSGD) files to generate a general linear model that considered age, sex, and years of education as covariates with DODS (different offset and different slope) settings. For emotional intelligence, we used two different statistical models for its total score (Eq. 2) and subscale scores (Eq. 3):

$$vIFC = r_{1} age + r_{2} sex + r_{3} edu + r_{4} EI + e$$
(2)
$$vIFC = r_{1} age + r_{2} sex + r_{3} edu + r_{4} EP + r_{5} SEM + r_{6} OEM + r_{7} EU + e$$
(3)

Similarly, for cognitive intelligence, we used three different models for full-scale intelligence (Eq. 4), two subscales (Eq. 5) and four subscales (Eq. 6) as follows:

$$vIFC = r_{1} age + r_{2} sex + r_{3} edu + r_{4} fIQ + e$$
(4)
$$vIFC = r_{1} age + r_{2} sex + r_{3} edu + r_{4} GAI + r_{5} CPI + r_{6} e$$
(5)
$$vIFC = r_{1} age + r_{2} sex + r_{3} edu + r_{4} VCI + r_{5} PRI + r_{6} WMI + r_{7} PSI + r_{8} e$$
(6)

Finally, the vertex-wise significance values for each contrast of group comparisons were corrected with the false discovery rate (FDR) method (FDR α = 0.05/2, corrected p = 0.05/2). After acquiring the clusters with significant vIFC-behavior correlations, we also plotted scatters for the residues of the average vIFC within the cluster and behavioral measurements (EI and fIQ) after controlling for age, sex, and years of education.

Connectivity distance

Distance has been conceived in the algorithm of D-vIFC. Intelligence and emotional intelligence could be related to the functional criticality of different spatial scales. Here, we defined another new measurement called connectivity distance to validate our D-vIFC analysis and further characterize the spatial scales of individual behaviors (Fig. 1). First, we designated clusters with significant correlations between D-vIFC and intelligence (or emotional intelligence) as sigCluster. Then, we calculated whole-brain functional connectivity by taking sigCluster as the seed region for each participant. After Bonferroni multiple comparison correction, we obtained some vertices with significant functional connectivity with sigCluster. Connectivity distance for each participant was defined as the average distance between sigCluster and those vertices with significant functional connectivity. Finally, we obtained the connectivity distance of vIFCi-IQ (i = 1–4, or vIFCi-EI) by averaging the distances of all the sigClusters corresponding to vIFCi versus IQ (EI).

Results

In this study, we used four smoothing sizes and four thresholds of connectivity (included in the definition of vIFC), and there were 16 sets of results for each statistical analysis. The effects of both smoothing and the threshold of connectivity on the final results were both small and gradually changed from one to one, and this type of consistency indirectly demonstrated the reliability of our algorithm and the analysis used in this study. The threshold of connectivity p4 produced pronounced correlations in almost all the analyses in addition to the correlation between vIFC1 and CPI in middle-aged individuals (p1). Here, we report the most pronounced results based on p4, 14 mm of spatial smoothing for emotional intelligence and 10 mm of spatial smoothing for cognitive intelligence. Apart from FDR multiple comparison correction of vertex-wise significance and Bonferroni correction for the two hemispheres, we also excluded the results with cluster sizes smaller than 5 vertices.

To provide an intuitive illustration of vIFC variations and a comparison with our previous neighborhood-based N-vIFC, we show the N-vIFC and D-vIFC of four different distance thresholds of one participant aged 25.6 years in the entire cortical mantle in Fig. 2. D-vIFC1, D-vIFC2, D-vIFC3 and D-vIFC4 exhibited very similar profiles. Similar to the original neighborhood-based functional criticality N-vIFC, the inferior parietal and precuneus exhibited large D-vIFCs. The normplots of the D-vIFC values showed that both the left end and the middle of the distribution followed a normal distribution, but only the middle part of N-vIFC followed a normal distribution. This conclusion was verified across all the participants. Our previous algorithm (N-vIFC) was based on a neighborhood in the definition of the ‘within-group’ cluster. The neighborhood only detected very local regions in the cortex, but distance-based vIFC (D-vIFC) could integrate more information at different spatial scales in terms of ‘distance’. D-vIFC values were more similar to a normal distribution and would better characterize individual differences in human brain and behaviors.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The N-vIFC and D-vIFC of one participant aged 25.6 years across the cortical mantle and their normplots

Intelligence and emotional intelligence were related to the functional criticality of separate brain regions for young adults

Figure 3 illustrates vertexwise significant positive correlations between vIFC and emotional intelligence after FDR corrections. Emotion utilization in emotional intelligence increased with vIFC1-4 in the right superior frontal gyrus for young adults. In contrast, intelligence was associated with all four vIFCs. Specifically, intelligence decreased with vIFC1, vIFC2, vIFC3 and vIFC4 in the left occipital-temporal sulcus for young adults, as shown in Fig. 4. Hence, intelligence and emotional intelligence mapped to separate brain regions and did not share any circuits in terms of brain mechanisms. For an intuitive illustration of vIFC at different spatial scales, intelligence and emotional intelligence scores, we also plotted partial correlation scatters for each significant finding as shown in Figs. 3 and 4. Note that we excluded nonsignificant partial correlation clusters in Fig. 3 (the right precentral sulcus) and Fig. 4 (the left orbital gyrus).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Vertexwise significant positive correlations between vIFC and emotional intelligence across the entire cortical mantle in young adults. Emotion utilization in emotional intelligence increased with vIFC1, vIFC2, vIFC3 and vIFC4 in the right superior frontal gyrus for young adults

Fig. 4
figure 4

Vertexwise significant negative correlations between vIFC and intelligence across the entire cortical mantle in young adults. Intelligence decreased with vIFC1, vIFC2, vIFC3 and vIFC4 in the left occipital-temporal sulcus in young adults

Age-related interactions in brain mechanisms of intelligence and emotional intelligence

Using two groups of different ages, we observed significant age-related interactions in brain mechanisms of intelligence and emotional intelligence. For young adults, we observed significant negative brain-intelligence correlations in the left occipital-temporal sulcus. For middle-aged adults, we observed significant positive brain-intelligence correlations in the right insular gyrus (Fig. 5, the middle frontal sulcus). Additionally, we only observed significant associations of the brain with emotional intelligence in young adults. Such different brain correlates of intelligence and emotional intelligence at different ages indicate different developmental trajectories of intelligence and emotional intelligence.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Vertexwise significant positive correlations between vIFC and intelligence across the entire cortical mantle in middle-aged individuals. Cognitive proficiency decreased with vIFC1 in the right insula in middle-aged individuals

Intelligence was associated with a wider distance range than emotional intelligence

Figure 6 shows the average connectivity distance for each significant brain-behavior association and provides a more direct validation of the differences between intelligence and emotional intelligence. Blue and red represent negative and positive correlations of vIFC and behaviors, respectively. Both emotional intelligence and cognitive intelligence were associated with all distances of functional criticality, vIFC1, vIFC2, vIFC3 and vIFC4, demonstrating that both intelligences were related to multiple spatial scales of brain functional organization. Additionally, when we assessed the clusters with significant vIFC-behavioral correlations in the context of the Yeo-7 network, the right superior frontal gyrus resided in the default mode network. However, the left occipital-temporal sulcus belonged to the visual network, and the right insular gyrus was a part of the ventral attention network. Consequently, intelligence was related not only to a wider distance range but also to more widespread functional hierarchies compared with emotional intelligence. We used two stars to represent significant differences at p < 0.001 in the two-sample t-test of connectivity distances. Different thresholds (vIFC1, vIFC2, vIFC3 and vIFC4) contributed to similar connectivity distances; middle-aged participants showed shorter connectivity distances than young adults. When we compared connectivity distances of emotional intelligence with cognitive intelligence, we found that cognitive intelligence had a longer connectivity distance (p = 0.002).

Fig. 6
figure 6

The average connectivity distance for each significant brain-behavior association. Blue and red represent negative and positive correlations of vIFC and behaviors, respectively. We used two stars to represent significant differences at p < 0.001 in the two-sample t-test of connectivity distances. Middle-aged participants exhibited shorter connectivity distances of functional criticality compared with young adults

Discussion

Using distance-based functional criticality of the human brain, our study confirmed significant correlations of functional criticality with intelligence as well as emotional intelligence. For young adults, emotional intelligence was positively associated with all distances of functional criticality, and cognitive intelligence was negatively associated with all distances of functional criticality. For middle-aged adults, we observed significantly positive correlations of intelligence with short distance functional criticality. Such different brain correlates of intelligence and emotional intelligence at different ages may indicate different developmental trajectories of intelligence and emotional intelligence. From the perspective of connectivity distance, intelligence was related to a wider distance range than emotional intelligence.

Intelligence and emotional intelligence were related to distance-based functional criticality of separate brain regions for young adults

In this study, we observed significant correlations of distance-based functional criticality with intelligence and emotional intelligence in separate brain regions. This observation not only demonstrated that both intelligence and emotional intelligence were related to critical transitions of human brain dynamics, again verifying vIFC as an efficient neuroimaging marker [19, 20], but also implied possible interactions of intelligence and emotional intelligence in terms of their brain mechanisms. First, intelligence was a hot topic even 100 years ago, and it has been related to brain size [39, 40], brain morphology [41, 42], brain functional measurements [43,44,45], and network topology of brain morphology and function, such as network efficiency and the small world [46, 47]. Although the concept of emotional intelligence is not completely accepted, it could represent a good supplement to cognitive intelligence. However, there have been few studies on the associations of intelligence/emotional intelligence with brain dynamics. Compared with neuronal avalanches mediated by structural heterogeneity of networks [48], functional criticality measures the probability of critical transitions of human brain dynamics at the spatial–temporal scale of millimeters and seconds acquired from fMRI. The correlations of intelligence/emotional intelligence with such abrupt changes in human brain dynamics may underlie the contributions of large amounts of neurons spiking to cognition and emotion. In addition, neighborhood-based vIFC has been successfully used to verify MCI as a transition state during AD progression [19] as well as associations with behavioral and physiological measurements [20]. Integrating important topological information distance and eliminating the limit of the neighborhood hypothesis, this study again confirmed functional criticality as an efficient and sensitive neuroimaging marker, thereby promoting its application in future studies on normal and abnormal populations. Second, for the young adult group, intelligence was related to the left occipital-temporal sulcus, and emotional intelligence was related to the right superior frontal gyrus. The occipital-temporal area was sensitive to the face [49], body [50], written Chinese [51], sustained visual attention [52] and multisensory integration [53]. These findings may be related to the comprehension capability of words and figures in intelligence tests. The frontal gyrus is part of the default mode network and is related to task switching [54] and cognitive control [55]. These were all higher-order cognitions and could be used to explain the comprehensive competence in the emotional intelligence test. If there were some overlapping brain regions for intelligence and emotional intelligence, the interactions between them could be explained. Previous studies have demonstrated some interactions between cognition and emotion [25, 26, 56]. However, our results did not show any overlapping regions here. The following reasons may explain these findings. There may indeed be very limited direct interactions between intelligence and emotional intelligence in terms of functional criticality. In addition, our sample size was too small and the number of participants should be increased to deduce more conclusions.

Age-related interactions in brain mechanisms of intelligence

First, we observed significant age-related interactions in the associations of functional criticality with intelligence. For young adults, intelligence was significantly negatively correlated with functional criticality in the left occipital-temporal sulcus. For middle-aged adults, intelligence was significantly positively correlated with functional criticality in the right insular gyrus. The inverse correlations of functional criticality with intelligence at different ages may be explained as follows. Young adults need fewer large changes in brain activity or metabolic cost to achieve cognitive intelligence; however, middle-aged adults require more large changes in brain activity or metabolic cost to achieve cognitive intelligence. Additionally, the occipital-temporal sulcus is more related to visual function [49,50,51,52], and the insular gyrus is more related to limbic and higher-order hierarchy across the entire cortex [57, 58]. These findings consistently indicated that the intelligence of middle-aged adults recruited more resources and higher hierarchy regions compared with young adults, and this conclusion was consistent with the developmental theory of the human brain: the more complicated brain regions, such as the prefrontal cortex, mature the latter [59]. Second, for young adults, intelligence was significantly correlated with all distance-based vIFCs. However, for middle-aged adults, intelligence was significantly correlated with short distance-based vIFCs. Such different distance dependence at different ages was consistent with previous human brain functional development studies that demonstrated that long-range connectivity was more sensitive to aging [60, 61]. Third, there were studies addressing age-related interactions in the associations of amygdala-frontal connectivity with emotional face processing [62]; there were also studies reporting age interactions in the associations of intelligence with electrical signals of the human brain [63] and reinforcing age correlations of intelligence with human brain morphology [64] and human brain functional connectivity [65]. However, no intelligence-related study has demonstrated inverse associations of intelligence with the human brain at different ages. Our study is the first to show a transition from a negative to a positive association of the human brain with intelligence. Actually, not only the development of intelligence and emotional intelligence [66] but also the associations of intelligence with the human brain are dynamic. Our study promotes the use of more specific age bins and gives full consideration to age effects in future human brain studies.

Intelligence was related to a wider distance range than emotional intelligence

Our study showed that intelligence was related to all distance-based vIFCs in young adults and short-distance vIFC1s in middle-aged adults, but emotional intelligence was only related to all distance-based vIFCs in young adults. Actually, there were only a few studies integrating intelligence and emotional intelligence together [21, 67, 68], and they only involved behavioral measurements but no brain mechanisms. Intelligence is a widely accepted and widely used construct in scientific research, whereas the concept of emotional intelligence is not widely accepted in the academic field. Intelligence comprised reason, plan, solve problem, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly and learn from experience. Hence, intelligence is a very complicated and complex concept. In contrast, emotional intelligence refers to social intelligence, and the notion of social intelligence as a dark intelligence [69], its dimensional structure [70], and its ability to moderate the relationship between stress and mental health have been assessed [71]. Given the definitions of intelligence and emotional intelligence, intelligence should be the basis of emotional intelligence. Therefore, intelligence was related to wider distance vIFC compared to emotional intelligence.

Furthermore, our study was the first to associate intelligence and emotional intelligence with distance, a quantitative measurement of human brain functional architecture. Intelligence was related to all distance-based functional criticality. This finding is consistent with findings from previous studies indicating that intelligence was associated with shorter characteristic path length [47] together with increased local information processing[72], long-distance theta coherence between frontal and parieto-occipital areas [73], and phase locking between short-distance regions of the frontal cortex [74]. However, there was no distance-related demonstration of emotional intelligence. Given that distance is a key factor in human brain functional organization, different distance dependences may characterize different psychological measurements, such as intelligence and emotional intelligence. Our study may promote distance as a quantitative marker for future psychological behavioral studies.

There were also some limitations in this study. The sample size has become increasingly important in fMRI studies, and our sample size was not large. Sample size may affect our results here, and in the future, big data should be used to verify the associations of distance-dependent vIFC with intelligence and emotional intelligence. Additionally, considering that criticality is a dynamic characteristic close to phase transition, measuring human brain criticality during the performance of tasks rather than resting-state fMRI would be very promising in future studies.

Conclusions

Our study confirmed that both emotional and cognitive intelligence were associated with functional criticality. For young adults, emotional intelligence was positively associated with all distances of functional criticality, and cognitive intelligence was negatively associated with all distances of functional criticality. For middle-aged adults, only cognitive intelligence was positively correlated with short distance functional criticality. For different age groups, our findings not only revealed an association between intelligence/emotional intelligence and functional criticality but also quantitatively characterized individual behaviors in terms of anatomical distance.

Availability of data and materials

https://github.com/jiangatbj/D-vIFC.

Abbreviations

rfMRI:

Resting state functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

D-vIFC:

Distance-based vertex-wise Index of Functional Criticality

DTI:

Diffusion Tensor Imaging

fMRI:

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

AD:

Alzheimer’s Disease

WAIS:

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

SSEIS:

Schutte Self-Report Emotional Intelligence scale

FSIQ (fIQ):

Full-scale intelligence quotient

GAI:

General ability index

VCI:

Verbal comprehension index

PRI:

Perceptual reasoning index

CPI:

Cognitive proficiency index

WMI:

Working memory index

PSI:

Processing speed index

MRI:

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

TR:

Repetition time

TE:

Echo time

FA:

Flip angle

EPI:

Echo planar imaging

CCS:

Connectome Computation System

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

WM:

White matter

GM:

Gray matter

ICA-AROMA:

Independent Component Analysis-based automatic removal of motion artifacts

FSL:

FMRI Software Library

AFNI:

Analysis of Functional Neuroimages

FSGD:

FreeSurfer Group Descriptor

DODS:

Different Offset and Different Slope

FDR:

False Discovery Rate

EI:

Emotional intelligence

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

mcBBR:

Minimal cost of Boundary-Based Registration

meanFD:

Mean (average) of Frame-wise Displacement

BOLD:

Blood oxygenation level dependent

PCC:

Pearson correlation coefficient

STD:

Standard deviation

sigCluster:

(Statistical) Significant cluster

GLM:

General linear model

N-vIFC:

Neighborhood-based vertex-wise Index of Functional Criticality

MCI:

Mild Cognitive Impairment

References

  1. Bullmore ET, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(3):186–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Sporns O, Betzel RF. Modular brain networks. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;67:613–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. He Y, Chen Z, Gong G, Evans A. Neuronal networks in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscientist. 2009;15(4):333–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bullmore ET, Sporns O. The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(5):336–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Gong G, He Y, Concha L, Lebel C, Gross DW, Evans AC, Beaulieu C. Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(3):524–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Zuo XN, Zhang Z, He Y, Zang Y. The human functional connectome: its methodology, developmental trajectory and behavioral association. Chin Sci Bull. 2012;57(35):3399–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Liao X, Vasilakos AV, He Y. Small-world human brain networks: perspectives and challenges. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;77:286–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ercsey-Ravasz M, Markov NT, Lamy C, Van Essen DC, Knoblauch K, Toroczkai Z, Kennedy H. A predictive network model of cerebral cortical connectivity based on a distance rule. Neuron. 2013;80(1):184–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Salvador R, Suckling J, Coleman MR, Pickard JD, Menon D, Bullmore ET. Neurophysiological architecture of functional magnetic resonance images of human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(9):1332–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sepulcre J, Liu H, Talukdar T, Martincorena I, Yeo BTT, Buckner RL. The organization of local and distant functional connectivity in the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6(6):e1000808.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Courchesne E, Pierce K. Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15(2):225–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Alexander-Bloch AF, Vertes PE, Stidd R, Lalonde F, Clasen L, Rapoport J, Giedd J, Bullmore ET, Gogtay N. The anatomical distance of functional connections predicts brain network topology in health and schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(1):127–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Guo S, Palaniyappan L, Yang B, Liu Z, Xue Z, Feng J. Anatomical distance affects functional connectivity in patients with schizophrenia and their siblings. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(2):449–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Guo W, Liu F, Xiao C, Liu J, Yu M, Zhang Z, Zhang J, Zhao J. Increased short-range and long-range functional connectivity in first-episode, medication-naive schizophrenia at rest. Schizophr Res. 2015;166(1–3):144–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cocchi L, Gollo LL, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. Criticality in the brain: a synthesis of neurobiology, models and cognition. Prog Neurobiol. 2017;158:132–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Beggs JM, Timme N. Being critical of criticality in the brain. Front Physiol. 2012;3:163.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Kitzbichler MG, Smith ML, Christensen SR, Bullmore ET. Broadband criticality of human brain network synchronization. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(3):e1000314.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Chen L, Liu R, Liu Z, Li M, Aihara K. Detecting early-warning signals for sudden deterioration of complex diseases by dynamical network biomarkers. Sci Rep. 2012;2:342.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Jiang L, Sui D, Qiao K, Dong H-M, Chen L, Han Y. Impaired functional criticality of human brain during Alzheimer’s disease progression. Sci Rep. 2018;8:1324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Jiang L, Qiao K, Sui D, Zhang Z, Dong H-M. Functional criticality in the human brain: Physiological, behavioral and neurodevelopmental correlates. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(3):e0213690.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Jausovec N, Jausovec K. Differences in induced gamma and upper alpha oscillations in the human brain related to verbal/performance and emotional intelligence. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005;56(3):223–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Jung RE, Haier RJ. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30(2):154–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Basten U, Hilger K, Fiebach CJ. Where smart brains are different: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence. 2015;51:10–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G. A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(9):396–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ursu S, Kring AM, Gard MG, Minzenberg MJ, Yoon JH, Ragland JD, Solomon M, Carter CS. Prefrontal cortical deficits and impaired cognition-emotion interactions in schizophrenia. Am J Psych. 2011;168(3):276–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Cromheeke S, Mueller SC. Probing emotional influences on cognitive control: an ALE meta-analysis of cognition emotion interactions. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;219(3):995–1008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Schutte NS, Malouff JM, Hall LE, Haggerty DJ, Cooper JT, Golden CJ, Dornheim L. Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Pers Individ Differ. 1998;25(2):167–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Salovey P, Mayer JD. Emotional intelligence. Imagin Cogn Pers. 1990;9(3):185–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Wang CK. The relationship between emotional intelligence and anxiety, depression and mood in a sample of college students. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2002;10:298–9.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Xu T, Yang Z, Jiang L, Xing X-X, Zuo X-N. A Connectome Computation System for discovery science of brain. Sci Bull. 2015;60(1):86–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Jiang L, Xu T, He Y, Hou X-H, Wang J, Cao X-Y, Wei G-X, Yang Z, He Y, Zuo X-N. Toward neurobiological characterization of functional homogeneity in the human cortex: regional variation, morphological association and functional covariance network organization. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220(5):2485–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis—I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):179–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis—II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):195–207.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Manjon JV, Coupe P. volBrain: an online MRI brain volumetry system. Front Neuroinform. 2016;10:30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Yan CG, Cheung B, Kelly C, Colcombe S, Craddock RC, Di Martino A, Li Q, Zuo X-N, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional connectomics. Neuroimage. 2013;76(1):183–201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Zuo XN, Xu T, Jiang L, Yang Z, Cao X-Y, He Y, Zang Y-F, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Toward reliable characterization of functional homogeneity in the human brain: preprocessing, scan duration, imaging resolution and computational space. Neuroimage. 2013;65:374–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage. 2009;48(1):63–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zoeller L, Polimeni JR, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Pietschnig J, Penke L, Wicherts JM, Zeiler M, Voracek M. Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: how strong are they and what do they mean? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;57:411–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. McDaniel MA. Big-brained people are smarter: a meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence. 2005;33(4):337–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Yang JJ, Yoon U, Yun HJ, Im K, Choi YY, Lee KH, Park H, Hough MG, Lee JM. Prediction for human intelligence using morphometric characteristics of cortical surface: partial least square analysis. Neuroscience. 2013;246:351–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Luders E, Narr KI, Bilder RM, Szeszko PR, Gurbani MN, Hamilton L, Toga AW, Gaser C. Mapping the relationship between cortical convolution and intelligence: effects of gender. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(9):2019–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lee KH, Choi YY, Gray JR, Cho SH, Chae JH, Lee S, Kim K. Neural correlates of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. Neuroimage. 2006;29(2):578–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Schultz DH, Cole MW. Higher intelligence is associated with less task-related brain network reconfiguration. J Neurosci. 2016;36(33):8551–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Vakhtin AA, Ryman SG, Flores RA, Jung RE. Functional brain networks contributing to the Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory of Intelligence. Neuroimage. 2014;103:349–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Langer N, Pedroni A, Gianotti LRR, Haenggi J, Knoch D, Jaencke L. Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(6):1393–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Li Y, Liu Y, Li J, Qin W, Li K, Yu C, Jiang T. Brain anatomical network and intelligence. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(5):e1000395.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Wu S, Zhang Y, Cui Y, Li H, Wang J, Guo L, Xia Y, Yao D, Xu P, Guo D. Heterogeneity of synaptic input connectivity regulates spike-based neuronal avalanches. Neural Netw. 2019;110:91–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Shao H, Weng X, He S. Functional organization of the face-sensitive areas in human occipital-temporal cortex. Neuroimage. 2017;157:129–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ishizu T, Noguchi A, Ito Y, Ayabe T, Kojima S. Motor activity and imagery modulate the body-selective region in the occipital-temporal area: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neurosci Lett. 2009;465(1):85–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Liu H, Jiang Y, Zhang B, Ma L, He S, Weng X. The orthographic sensitivity to written Chinese in the occipital-temporal cortex. Exp Brain Res. 2013;227(3):387–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Li Z, Coles CD, Lynch ME, Ma X, Peltier S, Hu X. Occipital-temporal reduction and sustained visual attention deficit in prenatal alcohol exposed adults. Brain Imaging Behav. 2008;2(1):39–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Beauchamp MS. See me, hear me, touch me: multisensory integration in lateral occipital-temporal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15(2):145–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Cutini S, Scatturin P, Menon E, Bisiacchi PS, Gamberini L, Zorzi M, Dell’Acqua R. Selective activation of the superior frontal gyrus in task-switching: an event-related fNIRS study. Neuroimage. 2008;42(2):945–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Li W, Qin W, Liu H, Fan L, Wang J, Jiang T, Yu C. Subregions of the human superior frontal gyrus and their connections. Neuroimage. 2013;78:46–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Ochsner KN, Phelps E. Emerging perspectives on emotion-cognition interactions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(8):317–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Singer T, Critchley HD, Preuschoff K. A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(8):334–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5–6):655–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Thomas KM. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol Psychol. 2000;54(1–3):241–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Aging and functional brain networks. Mol Psych. 2012;17(5):549–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Menon V. Typical and atypical development of functional human brain networks: insights from resting-state FMRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:21–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Wu M, Kujawa A, Lu LH, Fitzgerald DA, Klumpp H, Fitzgerald KD, Monk CS, Luan Phan K. Age-related changes in amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotional face processing from childhood into young adulthood. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(5):1684–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Stauder JEA, van der Molen MW, Molenaar PCM. Age, intelligence, and event-related brain potentials during late childhood: a longitudinal study. Intelligence. 2003;31(3):257–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Roman FJ, Morillo D, Estrada E, Escorial S, Karama S, Colom R. Brain-intelligence relationships across childhood and adolescence: a latent-variable approach. Intelligence. 2018;68:21–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK. Functional MRI evidence for disparate developmental processes underlying intelligence in boys and girls. Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):1366–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Fernandez-Berrocal P, Cabello R, Castillo R, Extremera N. Gender differences in emotional intelligence: the mediating effect of age. Behav Psychol. 2012;20(1):77–89.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Jausovec N, Jausovec K. Sex differences in brain activity related to general and emotional intelligence. Brain Cogn. 2005;59(3):277–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Cote S, Miners CTH. Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelligence, and job performance. Adm Sci Q. 2006;51(1):1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Nagler UKJ, Reiter KJ, Furtner MR, Rauthmann JF. Is there a “dark intelligence”? Emotional intelligence is used by dark personalities to emotionally manipulate others. Pers Individ Differ. 2015;76:232–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Petrides KV, Furnham A. On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence. Pers Individ Differ. 2000;29(2):313–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Ciarrochi J, Deane FP, Anderson S. Emotional intelligence moderates the relationship between stress and mental health. Pers Individ Differ. 2002;32(2):197–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Thatcher RW, Palmero-Soler E, North DM, Biver CJ. Intelligence and EEG measures of information flow: efficiency and homeostatic neuroplasticity. Sci Rep. 2016;6:38890.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Anokhin AP, Lutzenberger W, Birbaumer N. Spatiotemporal organization of brain dynamics and intelligence: an EEG study in adolescents. Int J Psychophysiol. 1999;33(3):259–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Neubauer AC, Fink A. Intelligence and neural efficiency: Measures of brain activation versus measures of functional connectivity in the brain. Intelligence. 2009;37(2):223–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (11674388) and the National Key Basic Research and Development (973) Program (2015CB351702). The funding bodies had no role in the design of the study, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or in the writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization: LJ; Methodology: LJ; Formal analysis and investigation: LJ, KQ, CL; Writing-review and editing: LJ, CL. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lili Jiang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The institutional review board of the Institute of Psychology Chinese Academy of Sciences approved this study, and written informed consent was obtained from individual participants prior to data acquisition.

Consent to publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jiang, L., Qiao, K. & Li, C. Distance-based functional criticality in the human brain: intelligence and emotional intelligence. BMC Bioinformatics 22, 32 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12859-021-03973-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12859-021-03973-4

Keywords